Figure 1 |
As far as consciousness is concerned this process might be expressed in one of the following ways.
Option 1: lock on to a tooth and hold that lock for a couple of hundred milliseconds or so; release attention and move eyes to the right; twist head to the right; lock on to the next tooth; release attention and so on. The twisting head bit probably being at the border of consciousness: one is aware after a while that the head has moved, but one is probably not conscious of each small, catching-up-with-eyes increment.
The problem is that given that the brain can only consciously attend to one thing at a time, how does it jump to the next thing? How does it know, how is it sure, for example, that the thing that it has jumped to is the next thing?
Option 2: make a tooth the object of attention, expand the object of attention to the right to include the next tooth, contract the object of attention from the left to exclude the first tooth, make the next tooth the object of attention. From where we associate to the way that a worm moves along the ground, with successive contractions and expansions of its body.
This formulation pushes the business of moving eyes and head about into the background and concentrates more on the subjective experience.
Figure 2 |
We have talked in the past of various counting tasks – see, for example, references 5 and 6 – and we will talk, in a post to come, more generally of some of the many different kinds of counting tasks, mostly visual, and the various difficulties encountered therein. Here we concentrate on this one.
Figure 3 |
We then suppose that the brain is randomly generating saccades along the bar, giving us something like a one dimensional random walk. But there are biases. First, the eyes tend to jump to the right so as to reconnoitre the way ahead. Second, the eyes keeping jumping back to the tooth that is being attended to, or teeth in the case of the second option above, the one in each of the two foveae when looking straight ahead. The eyes need to spend quality time on the tooth which is the subject of conscious attention in order to keep the subjective experience up to strength, up to snuff.
After each saccade, the brain decides whether it is looking at tooth or not tooth.
Figure 4 |
After a further while, the brain might decide that the teeth are arranged in an orderly way and need less additional input to compute the next saccade.
All this is happening more or less subconsciously, with the conscious attention remaining locked on to the tooth which we have marked with the red spot.
Implications for LWS-N
The LWS-N proposition – for which see reference 3 - is that consciousness is organised into a succession of frames, each lasting of the order of a second or so. Frames can be updated, but for big updates the brain starts over. We talk of frames being compiled, by analogy with the computer programs of the 1980’s.
The visual part of consciousness will be held on one or more layers of LWS-N, with the teeth of the present note probably be represented as a mixture of layer objects (the current teeth, the important teeth) and parts of the layer objects (the rest).
We had thought that the object being attended to will be marked, inter alia, by a column object linking the visual part of that attention to other parts.
As our attention moves from one tooth to the next, at the very least that column object is going to have to be moved and during that movement, while there may be attention, there will not be a single tooth which is the object of that attention.
Figure 5 |
In this case, provided there is enough information in the visual layer, the shift from tooth to tooth can be managed by shifting the activation processes, as suggested in the figure above. In the middle of this process, the visual detail of the teeth may be weakened, with the subjective experience focussed just on the fact of there being two teeth and the need to move from one to the other. From where we associate to Tononi and Koch’s talk at reference 7 of our experience being both integrated and differentiated, which may, in part, be grasping at the same problem.
All this being implemented by large numbers of neurons firing away, possibly as many as hundreds of thousands of them, under the hood, as it were.
From time to time, the visual layer will need to be refreshed, a new frame will need to be built and it is, perhaps, at these times that it is going to be particularly easy to lose one’s place in the row of teeth.
Testing
Figure 6 |
With the figure above suggesting the sort of thing that eye trackers can produce, taken, via Wikipedia, from reference 4. ‘This study by Yarbus (1967) is often referred to as evidence on how the task given to a person influences his or her eye movement’. No doubt things have moved on since then.
Testing of LWS-N being rather more difficult, our not yet having any very clear idea of where it is – quite apart from the difficulty of recording small electrical affairs deep inside the living, human brain.
Conclusions
We have offered a speculation about how the brain, while only attending to one thing at a time, can jump in an orderly way from one thing to the next.
References
Reference 1: https://imotions.com/.
Reference 2: The fading of stabilized images: Eye movements and information processing - Stanley Coren, Clare Porac – 1974.
Reference 3: http://psmv3.blogspot.com/2018/05/an-update-on-seeing-red-rectangles.html. A slightly out of date portal to the world of LWS-N.
Reference 4: Eye movements and vision - Yarbus, A. L. – 1967.
Reference 5: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2019/03/counting.html.
Reference 6: https://psmv3.blogspot.com/2018/09/shopping-lists.html. Second part is about counting the floors of Vauxhall Tower.
Reference 7: Consciousness: here, there and everywhere - Tononi & Koch – 2015.
Reference 8: Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Codes Our Thoughts - Stanislas Dehaene – 2016.
Group search key: srd.
No comments:
Post a Comment