Wednesday, 30 September 2020

The suitcase

A short while I ago I read and liked the book by Dovlatov at reference 2 noticed at reference 1 - and so thought that I would try another, the one about a suitcase at reference 3.

Procured through Abebooks France through RecycLivre who got it from Caen public library. Probably the one snapped above, built in 1971 or so, with the main operation now having been moved to a flashy new building on the docks. On my laptop, I thought I could make out books behind the glass and there is also the tell-tale 'Bibl' above the glass. Place Louis Guillouard. Perhaps the flashy new building dumped a lot of old stock to make room for multi-media events and other stuff of that sort.

Inside I learn that French libraries annul books when they retire them, while we just withdraw them. The same sort of heavy duty, public service stamp in black though. There is also an instruction stamped in red asking readers not to attempt to repair damaged books, rather to report damage to a librarian. And I have to say that this second book, while from the same publisher as the first one, is in much poorer condition. Perhaps it was a much read book in its day, although there is no stamped record of loans. I don't suppose that they have done that for years, any more than we have.

And while it might have been much read, I did not like it as much as the first. Constructed on very similar lines, essentially eight short stories, each one about an item of clothing in the suitcase the author took with him when he left the Soviet Union. While the first was twelve short stories linked together in a different way. But very much the same kind of story, stories about the sometimes dreary life of a marginal journalist during the twilight years of the Soviet Union. Another common thread was the amount of drink taken. But somehow, it just seemed to be going over the same ground. Perhaps if I could read the original, or if my French had been better, I would have got on better. Perhaps I shall try again in a few months time.

Reference 1: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/rubbing-along.html.

Reference 2: Le Compromis - Sergueϊ Dovlatov - 1981/2005.

Reference 3: La Valise - Sergueϊ Dovlatov - 1986/2001.

Reference 4: https://www.recyclivre.com/.

Reference 5: https://bibliotheques.caenlamer.fr/caen-bibliotheque-alexis-de-tocqueville-accueil.aspx. Flashy new library.

Missing

When I was young, I thought I knew something about logic, spent quality time on it. I also read about earnest undergraduates of the 1930's who spent their time in the pub arguing about an eminence called Wittgenstein. But I never, at least as far as I can remember, heard about him in lectures or otherwise troubled myself about him.

Then a few days ago, in reference 2, Langer spends some pages on him and I thought it was time to repair this hole in my education and turned to Abebooks. Who turned up a respectable copy of Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus from Routledge & Kegan Paul, from 1972. 

A work which has gone through many reprints, corrections and translations since it origins in 1921. One supposes that it was very much the thing in Langer's formative years.

With this copy containing a preface by Bertrand Russell and a parallel text in German and English of Tractatus itself. Presumably put together at a time when respectable philosophers, logicians and mathematicians were expected to know German, even if they were English.

We will see if I get beyond opening the book, beyond the opening sentence: 'The world is all that is the case'. All very biblical, all very gospel according to St. John.

Reference 1: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Ludwig_Wittgenstein. A chap whom I now know to have had an interesting life.

Reference 2: Philosophy in a new key: A Study in the Symbolism of Reason, Rite, and Art - Langer, S. K. – 1942.

Tuesday, 29 September 2020

Series 3, Episode VII

The slowdown on the brick walks continues, but they are still just about there and have now reached a total of 3,408 bricks, 453,264 horizontal metres and 10,224 vertical metres. Applying the modulo 8,000 rule, we found a mountain in Norway at 2,222 metres which is near enough.

Storebjørn, literally Big Bear from the likeness of the main and subordinate peaks to the ears of a bear if viewed from the right direction, is a mountain in Lom, Innlandet, Norway. Part of the Jotunheimen mountain range.

Having passed on the Carpathians (see reference 2), Polly decided on a late break and since Pedro and Yuri said that they would rather stay in the warm with their computer games, she took her new friend the kitten with her to carry her suitcase. Never know what you might need up a mountain. Such a new friend that she has not yet decided on a name, although she says she has several candidate names - which she is keeping to herself for now.

Snapped here just before starting the big push up this north facing snow slope to the summit. Kitten still full of beans even if Polly is looking a bit serious.

Snapped here from gmaps. The red marker is the mountain in question, while Lom seems to be a little to the north east, underneath where it says 'Fossbergom'. Not quite the end of the world as the man from Street View has been by. No MacDonald's that I could see, but plenty of other eating, drinking and sleeping opportunities.

Reference 1: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/08/series-3-episode-vi.html.

Reference 2: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/08/the-brick-scene.html.

Group search key: wwwy.

Problems


I was reminded this morning of how silly our problems in Northern Ireland must seem from some other parts of the world which have far more complex problems to solve. Reminded by reading of trouble flaring up again between Armenia and Azerbaijan, trouble rooted in the collapse of the Ottoman Empire in 1918 and the rather later collapse of the Soviet Empire, heir to that of the Romanovs. Not for nothing did Stalin make his theoretical début on the problem of nationalities. I think his answer was let them have their funny languages and fancy costumes - perhaps even to run around the hills with their antiquated muskets - provided that they toed the Party line from Moscow.

The first snap above is taken from reference 1 on the occasion of a previous flare-up in 2016: '... when His Holiness Karekin II, the Supreme Patriarch and Catholicos of All Armenians, conducted an unprecedented prayer at the St. Gregory the Illuminator Church in Yerevan – for the Homeland and the soldiers defending it...'.

The second is Bing's best effort at a map of this troubled area, with Azerbaijan having a southern enclave bordering Iran and a large disputed area on the disputed border with Armenia. Important oil and gas pipelines running from the Caspian, through Azerbaijan, Georgia and Turkey are not shown. Pipelines which provide us in the west with some leverage against Putin, a major supplier in that department. So we don't really want a row with Azerbaijan.

While the chunk of Armenia which appears to have been given back to Turkey reminds me of the Armenian massacres around the time of the first world war. Documented at reference 2, read in 2007 and now largely forgotten. Apart from a memory that all this was a dreadful business which should not be forgotten, on the strength of which the book has survived many culls. I could even put my hand on it this morning, perhaps to be looked at, if not read, again.

In the course of all this, I turned up another theoretical endeavour, as noticed at reference 4. More useful background.

Reference 1: https://www.armenianow.com/en/.

Reference 2: A shameful act - Taner Akҫam - 2006.

Reference 3: http://pumpkinstrokemarrow.blogspot.com/2007/06/so-what-are-walls-for.html. The only trace on the blog of the foregoing.

Reference 4: http://psmv3.blogspot.com/2018/09/do-we-want-to-be-tight-or-loose.html.

Monday, 28 September 2020

Everest again

This afternoon, Microsoft News (for once) brings me an interesting article (reference 1) about measuring the height of Everest. Coincidentally, on the very same afternoon, the NYRB brings me notice of yet another book about the glory days of the climbing of Everest, roughly concentrated on the period 1930-1950 – and ending with John Hunt leading a successful ascent in 1953. A job for which the otherwise very well qualified Eric Shipton was rejected on the grounds of not being aggressive enough, not being fanatical enough. A story dominated, if not monopolised, by white Caucasian males. Will I fall for it?

But today’s story, something of a extended reprise of that at reference 5, is about finding out how high Everest is.

First we have a team of Nepalese surveyors lugging the necessary equipment up to the top of Everest in May last year and spending a couple of hours up there with it, talking to satellites and such like. In the pitch dark, in the small hours of the morning. When most other people stay just long enough to take a picture and then head on down while they still can.

But then, what do we mean by height? Height above sea level is all very well, but the sea level varies from place to place, from time to time. How do we set the standard?

Or should we take a different tack altogether and measure from the centre of the earth – a proceeding which puts a much smaller mountain called Chimborazo, in Ecuador, at the top of the list?

And anyway, surely it is the height one has to climb on the day that counts. Can’t they come up with some way of scoring height from base camps? 

Against which one might argue that that would not give fair weight to the problems of climbing in thin air. Can’t they come up with some way of measuring down from somewhere up above, rather than up from somewhere down below?

A minor problem about whether the snow and ice at the top of Everest counts as part of the height.

And quite apart from all these difficulties, Everest moves around. Generally it moves slowly up as the Indian subcontinent presses up against Asia. Then one has earthquakes from time to time, one of which moved the summit down by a couple of feet in 1934. For which see reference 6.

Enough to keep the surveyors and the surveying standards people busy for years and years to come.

PS 1: or, as one might say, its all relative.

PS 2: BH's response was rather sexist. 'Only men would get into such a lather about exactly how high a mountain was. Sensible people would settle for it just being high'. While T. E. Lawrence attributed a similar sentiment, to do with stars, to Auda abu Tayi. Eastist rather than sexist in this case. See reference 7.

References

Reference 1: How do you measure Everest? It's complicated by frostbite and politics – Freddie Wilkinson/National Geographic – 2020. Brought to me by Microsoft News.

Reference 2: Because It’s Still There – Joshua Hammer/NYRB – 2020.

Reference 3: The World Beneath Their Feet: Mountaineering, Madness, and the Deadly Race to Summit the Himalayas – Scott Ellsworth – 2021. Perhaps the NYRB get a pre-publication copy for review.

Reference 4: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2019/05/stupidity.html. The most recent, relevant notice of Everest. Various trivia since then.

Reference 5: http://psmv3.blogspot.com/2017/02/mondays-factlet.html. I did once know about Chimborazo before.

Reference 6: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/1934_Nepal%E2%80%93India_earthquake

Reference 7: http://psmv2.blogspot.com/2015/06/the-garden-of-eden.html.

Wellingtonia 18

A stray Wellingtonia at the southern entrance to the pinetum which occupies a chunk of the northern peninsular of the RHS garden at Wisley. I couldn't find a label and I couldn't get at the other side to take a photograph with the sun behind me. As it is, the sun is hidden behind the trunk; adequate rather than good.

Fairly sure that it is a young Wellingtonia. The shape of the tree is about right and the trunk splays out at the base. But in the absence of a label, I am not completely sure - so revision may be necessary in due course.

Reference 1: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/wellingtonia-17.html.

Group search key: wgc, wsc.

Sunday, 27 September 2020

Afternoon tea at the hatch

Denbies is a wine estate near Dorking with lots of facilities and lots of space, not least for the parking of cars. And like many rural industries - not to mention garden centres - they make the most of it. One wonders what proportion of their income actually comes from selling wine, but that I have yet to investigate. Would I be able to deduce this from accounts filed at Companies House? Accounts which I often find hard to read.

So given the risks around urban activities just presently, last week we thought we would take tea at their hatch. This being an arrangement whereby you can buy your tea and cakes and so on at the hatch and then consume them at the chairs and tables provided. Complete with tapes marking out the one-way system.

A hatch which is now closed under the latest regulations? Or do they escape, being outdoors?

Served by a pert young miss, perhaps on furlough from her regular work in some club or bar, but pleasant enough. Tea, something called rhubarb and custard cake plus a glass of something called Surrey Gold, a nod to the place being a wine estate.

The cake being florid in appearance, sweet and entirely eatable, very much the sort of thing I associate with Coughlans of reference 3 and their outlet at Horton Retail. An outlet I used to use occasionally when I made more use of Horton Lane than I do now. Wine much better than I was expecting - with my expectations of English wine being low. Not the right climate for booze you know.

BH paid a visit to the tastefully fitted out shop and I took the opportunity to buy three bottles of Surrey Gold, at about the same price as the Villa Maria sauvignon blanc I used to buy from Majestic.

From there to the farm shop adjacent where they had a splendid display of all kinds of tomatoes, occupying both sides of a brand new market barrow. I thought about, but decided against, scoring it as a fake, which seemed a little unkind.

The tomato people - Nutbourne Nursery in Pulborough - don't seem to have a web site but they do have plenty of glass. The tomatoes might actually be grown there, not bought in from some wholesale tomato operation out of Nine Elms. In our case, two very large tomatoes, the sort of thing we used to buy in the days when we used to camp in France. At about £2.22 a pop, BH not best pleased, it coming out of her account.

Out to check that the plastic cows were still in place, which they were. Last noticed at reference 2.

I took a walk down one of the rows of grapes to see what there was to see. With the answer being not very much. You didn't seem to get a huge amount of grape given the number of vines and the amount of space taken. But BH told me afterwards that it was an early harvest, already half picked, so perhaps my row had had a preliminary picking.

On the way home, puzzled by what looked like a lot of dead trees on the sides of Box Hill. Not very big, but surely not box trees, fallen prey to blight or caterpillars? Has the lack of rain done for some of these trees on chalk? Chalk drying out a lot fast than the clay we are on here at Epsom. Neither Bing nor Google turn anything up, so perhaps a return visit is indicated.


The following day I set to to make a French style stew with onion, tomato and saucisson sec, this last being from Sainsbury's. Tomatoes were rather odd inside, reminding me of the inside of kidneys.

A couple of hours later, I was very pleased with the result, quite different from regular, supermarket tomatoes. Well worth the extra. With tinned tomatoes being somewhere else again - tinned tomatoes being one of my pet aversions. Never cook with them.

Rounded out with a green salad, taken without oil or vinegar. Proper English salad.

One portion of stew left over, now frozen against some future ready-meal need.

PS 1: in the event, Bing turned up the Denbies records at Companies House in next to no time. From which it seems that they became a private limited company in 2019, which perhaps means that their accounts remain private. But there are a couple of documents relating to charges on the estate arising from loans or loan facilities from HSBC. And apart from the comings and goings of various important people, that is about it.

PS 2: their web site reveals that Surrey Gold is 'a blend of Müller-Thurgau, Ortega and Bacchus, this has a herbal, fruity, citric quality, with lovely fresh elderflower aromas. Fresh, clean'. None of this sauvignon blanc carry on. While I can spend a good deal more on something called Brokes Botrytis Ortega 2016, 'made from a specially-selected parcel of Ortega, this Vineyard Select wine is a deep golden colour with notes of vanilla...'. Maybe.

Reference 1: https://www.denbies.co.uk/.

Reference 2: http://psmv4.blogspot.com/2019/09/pit-stop.html. Our last visit was just about a year ago. Perhaps they get themselves into the media around the time of the grape harvest and we pick up on that.

Reference 3: http://www.coughlansbakery.co.uk/.

Reference 4: https://psmv3.blogspot.com/search?q=coughlans. The record of my use of same. Cakes inspected but bread bought.

Conundrum

At around 1020 this morning I get a text message from the NHS test and trace people, inviting me to download their shiny new app from Apple or Google.

Sadly, as far as I can make out, this does not play on my Microsoft mobile phone which runs on Windows. Furthermore, when I checked the other day, I think the story was that it does play not on a lot of other, older phones.

So Microsoft have not done whatever it would take to convert the Apple-Google effort so that it would run on their phones. Which, to be fair, have been discontinued and are probably more or less on life-support rather than on regular support - all of which probably means that cranking up up a whole new app on them would be expensive. Quite possibly not justified for the numbers involved, not that I have any idea what they might be.

On the other hand, the Bill & Melinda Foundation has given a great deal of money to other aspects of the Covid effort. Perhaps as much as $500m.

Is all this enough to push me off my Microsoft phone - which I like and have got used to - and spend the money to move somewhere else, somewhere more app friendly? The £500 or so? Plus all the bother?

PS: I had been meaning to visit the shiny new Microsoft shop at Oxford Circus to see what they had to say about moving somewhere else, but that has now been on hold for six months.

Reference 1: https://covid19.nhs.uk/.

Reference 2: https://www.gatesfoundation.org/.

Saturday, 26 September 2020

The Lone Star State

In the margins of news about a death dealing amoeba - some relative of that same animal which featured in biology classes at my secondary school - I have learn some interesting facts about Texas.

First, that the water authority concerned, the people at reference 1, is a public authority of some kind. So in the country where they don't really believe in public services, at least some of their water supply is in public hands, unlike in this country where we still pay lip service to public services, especially of the health variety - and where everything to do with water was sold into privatisation years ago.

A public authority which appears to have annual public meetings at which important deeds are approved. Or not, as the case may be.

They also see fit to have a special charter which appears to cover the rights of the people owning land which some public authority covets. This can be seen at reference 2. I dare say we have rules of the same sort, but we don't draw attention to them in quite this way.

Second, I have learned something about the birth of Texas. I had thought that the Yankee imperialists just took it from the peace loving Mexicans. It now seems that Texas seceded from Mexico in 1835 or so and joined the Union in 1845 or so, as the 28th State. So not quite the land grab I had thought. 

But it may also be true that Yankees colonised a thinly populated Texas prior to 1835 and then drove the independence movement. So a land grab after all. I associate to the colonisation of what was then Palestine a hundred years later.

I may get around to finding out more about the history of Texas and its people.

In the meantime, a quick peek at my grand Times Atlas (from 1968), reveals that Texas is very much in two parts. The hilly two thirds to the west and the low lying third to the east, with the split running pretty much north-south, from Fort Worth down to Laredo. Also that Texas has its very own Colorado River, a lot bigger than our Thames but a lot smaller than the other Colorado, the one with the big dam. While the rather larger Red River marks the northern boundary with Oklahoma before nipping across Louisiana to join the Mississippi just north of New Orleans. I wonder if 'Colorado' is Spanish for 'Red'? But just as well to have different names for these two very different rivers.

Reference 1: http://brazosportwaterauthority.org/.

Reference 2: http://brazosportwaterauthority.org/images/landowners_billofrights%20english.pdf.

Reference 3: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Colorado_River_(Texas).

Manhattan

Reference 1 being a rather strange - and I believe commercially very successful - story from Simenon. And Bing knows all about a film from 1965. No idea if that was the first or only one. Set in New York and written shortly after he arrived in North America in 1945 (France not being very sure about his war record) and was living at a place called Sainte-Marguerite-du-Lac-Masson, a small place about 50km north west of Montreal. One supposes that Simenon knew New York well enough from visits.

The story of a middle aged French actor, newly divorced, by no means broke but living in reduced circumstances in a small flat in New York, who meets a French-ish lady desperate for somewhere to stay for the night in a night café. Various adventures are squashed into a week or so. She goes off to Mexico to attend to her sick daughter, then with her Hungarian diplomat father. She comes back and the two of them sail off into the sunset, prodigiously in love.

The sort of adventure of which I have little understanding and even less experience. I found the often wounding behaviour of the hero very strange. Both hero and heroine appeared to be heavy smokers and reasonably heavy drinkers. A story which can be read as a spin-off from his meeting Denyse Ouimet in New York in 1945, she being the lady who later became his second wife. A marriage born in passion which ended in tears, back in Europe, some fifteen years later. I dare say I will read the story again at some point. Will I fall for the film?

Book shop in blue

The next street

What looks like a very old church at the bottom of the street

A very decent, if inexpensive, hardback from Presses de la Cité from 1960. Beautifully wrapped and addressed, for the very first time, to 'Monsieur James Toller'. I had bought it from a small bookshop in Vallon en Sully in the middle of France, via eBay France. On the Cher, and as it happens a little to the west of Moulins, the nearest big town to where Maigret was born and raised. Otherwise, half way between Tours and Lyon.

A provincial France which I don't suppose I am ever going to get to see now. Which is a pity.

PS: interesting Google Street View camera artefact across the spire of the church. Yet to work out what has happened there.

Reference 1: Trois Chambres à Manhattan - Georges Simenon - 1946.

Reference 2: Petit Evelyne, Bouquiniste - 3 rue Marcel Gaumy, 03190 Vallon en Sully, France.

No.22

No.22 was spotted on a small car, round and red, as I crested the hill coming up to the roundabout at the Bridge Road/Hook Road Junction. Not the place to take a photograph and I didn't get a good look at the maker's plaque, but possibly a Peugeot 208. Just about a week since No.21, noticed at reference 1.

Cold breeze, mainly northerly, although direction did seem to depend on the direction one was travelling. I was glad to have scarf, gloves and woolly under my jacket, but plenty of people were out and about with much less on. Men in shorts. Ladies with bare arms. And one young lady, perhaps 16 or 17, in Cox Lane, in Lycra shorts, bare legs, and a very skimpy white top, arms and middle showing. Must have had a youthful circulation. Or the youthful new fashion imperative.

Back in Epsom, what sounded like the Surrey police helicopter, deployed for the second day running.

PS 1: Cox Lane being what comes after Jubilee Way when running anti-clockwise.

PS 2: with an advertisement for Vauxhall Corsa's appearing in my gmail in-box just seconds after I had asked Bing whether the car in question was a Vauxhall Corsa. I had already decided that it was not.

PS 3: there was a piece in yesterday's FT about a polio epidemic in Cork in the 1950's. Similar to our present difficulties in the sense that polio is a very infectious disease which is asymptomatic nearly all the time - but which can result in long term damage or death. And a big difference was that it mainly affected very young people, rather than very old people. I can't now find the FT piece, but offer reference 2 instead.

PS 4: there is also a postscript to the remarks in reference 3 about the poverty of the conscious experience, about its riches not standing up to inspection. As I came up the hill to the roundabout, I was not thinking about car number plates at all. As far as I can remember, I was not looking out for them. Nevertheless, the brain was on the case, knew which number I was on, and pushed the red car with its number into consciousness. Evidence, if more were needed, that the unconscious is busy with all kinds of stuff.

Reference 1: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/progress.html.

Reference 2: https://www.historyireland.com/20th-century-contemporary-history/the-1956-polio-epidemic-in-cork/.

Reference 3: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/pennies-and-elephants.html.

Friday, 25 September 2020

Haw jelly

About a week ago now, BH decided that I was bored and needed an activity for the afternoon. Her eyes lighted upon the small hawthorn tree in our front garden, about to deposit its load of haws on the pavement and came up with the notion that I should pick the haws. It being quite possible that there were more than usual, it having been a good April for the flowers, as noticed at reference 1.

First boil

A plus point was the opportunity to deploy the new-to-me steps from Cornwall, noticed at reference 2, which turned out rather well, managing to be both light and steady. Furthermore, a light weight stainless bowl fitted neatly on the top stage, at about 15 inches wide proving an excellent receptacle. One could just drop the fruit from on high and most of the time it found its way in. A bit like picking black currants, except that both the haws and the clusters were a bit smaller. And one needed to be careful not to pull the twigs off with the haws. But after a while one's hands adjusted.

About an hour or so later, I had a pound and a half of haws. Meanwhile, BH had turned up haw jelly in Dorothy Hartley of reference 3, a book given her by my father, a long time ago now. With, according to Wikipedia, Hartley having served time as an advisor to our long-running rustic soap, 'The Archers'. So the first step was to boil them up with a little water, applying the potato masher from time to time.

Drip time

Next step, out with the jelly bag and, once filled with the mash, suspend from beam erected across the kitchen. Got to make a bit of a splash. With the bowl being the bowl mentioned above. A certain amount of hot bag squeezing to help things along. Left overnight.

Second boil: part I

About a pint and a half of liquor, so about a pound and a half of sugar. With our fine sauté pan from Marylebone doing service as a jam pan, which last are long retired. I associate to the days when we used a heavy copper jam pan, long since criminalised.

Second boil: part II

On to the second boil, removing a certain amount of froth, which seems to include all the stuff which makes the brew cloudy. Gell testing plate visible left, which a neighbour told us afterwards would have been better cooled down in the fridge. Get a better test that way.

The result

The result looked well enough, nice and clear, but sadly, had not set overnight. I had not got the gell test right. Notwithstanding, the egg cup full did rather well in porridge. Being liquid worked much better than the rather firm crab apple jelly noticed at reference 6. Although yesterday, BH went to the bother of turning some of that jelly into jam sauce - heating up the jelly with a little water and lemon juice - which worked much better with porridge than neat jelly.

Testing plate visible behind, complete with smears and splodges. BH rather likes to eat the scum, aka froth. Four jam jars - lately used for storing old screws and nails in the garage - clearly a bit optimistic.

We worried that liquid jelly would not keep, so onto the second boiling. With the bubbles being quite different to those snapped above. Something was clearly going on as the excess water boiled off. I stayed nearby, being a little concerned that it would froth over, in the way of boiling milk. A frothing over onto a hot hob which would have taken a good bit of cleaning up, never mind the waste involved.

Second outcome

Still didn't pass the gell test in a convincing way, but it looked right. And this time it did indeed set, having lost getting on for half its volume, after due allowance for sticky waste along the way. 

It will be interesting to see whether we bother with this particular sort of jam again. Probably not a good thing for us pensioners to use a lot of it, it being one of the most concentrated forms of sugar known to man, but it does do rather well on porridge and steamed jam puddings. And perhaps we ought to steer clear of butter and jam on toast.

Mother's Ruin

PS: on the facing page of Hartley, we had a cryptic reference to the use of a concoction of sloe gin, penny royal and valerian by country wives in 'connubial emergencies'. Diligent search at the time failed to confirm the notion that this was a 1950's style, covert allusion to abortion. While Wikipedia reveals this morning that penny royal, aka mentha pulegium, was indeed a folk remedy used as an abortifacient. A rural version of the potions advertised below.

Discretion assured

Trumpists and right-to-lifeists take note. The sort of people who hang out at reference 4. I had not realised until this morning they that as well as being opposed to start-of-life action, they are also opposed to end-of-life action, that is to say assisted dying. For which see reference 5.

Reference 1: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/04/good-year.html.

Reference 2: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/ladder-nostalgia.html.

Reference 3: Food in England - Dorothy Hartley - 1954.

Reference 4: https://www.nrlc.org/.

Reference 5: https://www.dignityindying.org.uk/.

Reference 6: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/08/the-crabs-continued.html.

Pennies and elephants

 Contents

  • Introduction
  • A digression
  • Elephants
  • Some theory
  • Other devices
  • What does the subject bring to the party?
  • Other matters
  • Conclusions
  • References

Introduction

Something of a medley, but started off by thinking about seeing a near small object largely occluding a far big object, perhaps an old penny occluding an elephant. How does LWS-R deal with this? What gets projected onto LWS-R’s patch of cortex, and from there into consciousness?

An seemingly unlikely spin-off from perusal of reference 8, about the unlikely possibility of being conscious while not being conscious of anything, to which we shall return in due course. While for LWS-R, see reference 1.

Note that, for present purposes, an important property of LWS-R is that it is self contained; it includes everything which gets into consciousness, an everything which has been put there by the compiler and there is no need or possibility of referring out within the context of a frame. Everything has to be expressed by layer objects in the layers of LWS-R, supplemented by the links provided by column objects – for which see reference 6. While the compiler, which came before, did free-range, could and probably did draw on material from all over the brain.

A digression

Before getting onto elephants, we pause to think about trees.

Suppose we are in a state of relaxed repose, perhaps after some physical exercise. Just comfortably sitting in a garden chair, outside, gazing at the scene in front of one. We suppose a more or less natural scene, perhaps with grass, bushes, trees and some sky. A predominantly green scene; a scene in which things are growing, rather than having been made.

The eyes flicker about as our attention flickers around the scene. Lighting first on this spot, then that, without stopping anywhere very long.

Sometimes, perhaps for some considerable time, there are no conscious thoughts at all, although one is perfectly awake, ready for action if need be – and would be able to answer questions about that scene. Some without thinking, some needing one to take another look. To decide, perhaps, what sort of tree one was looking at.

No conscious thoughts in the sense of inner thoughts, typically in the form of words which do not make it all the way to the vocal apparatus of throat and mouth. For which see reference 14.

Furthermore, it might well be that the brain knew, in some sense, that one was looking at a beech tree – but did not see fit to intrude on consciousness with this fact. One was not conscious of looking a at beech tree.

One might be looking at the pattern of the branches holding up the canopy of the tree. One might be tracking the branches up and down. Or admiring the pattern that they made. Or one might be watching the rhythmic movements of the branches in the wind. And again, there would be nothing much in consciousness except the tracking, admiring or watching itself. The visual experience would be sufficient unto itself, it would not need supplementing with botanical or any other knowledge.

Not even the name of colour of the leaves, a strong and reasonably uniform green. The word ‘green’ is not drifting through the brain as inner thought from time to time. One might even pause for a second or two if asked the main colour of the scene. Information which one can get fast enough, but which is not in consciousness at the time of the question. It might be somewhere in the LWS-R data structure as a matter of computational convenience, but it would not have been activated, projected into the subjective experience.

We associate here to Chater’s argument at reference 10 that not much information is held in consciousness at any one time. Much less than one might at first think. Also to the tricks one can play with people by changing big things in full view, but which they are not attending to, without their noticing the change. And from there to the rather noisy video at reference 11.

Figure 1

Rather different, we might be in the room above, provided by a shop selling interior design. We try to sit quietly, our eyes flicking about the room. Here, many of the things the eyes light on have names usually in the form of common nouns, perhaps short phrases or clauses built around common nouns, which was not the case with the tree. But generally speaking, the names do not come to mind.

The brain may well being working away in the background, working out what things are and whether this supporting information needs to be brought to the subject’s attention. Perhaps a thought such as ‘don’t much like blue glass’ flits through the mind, being attached, at least temporarily, to the table lamp left on the visual layer.

But for most of the time, the brain is content with the unadorned visual layer in LWS-R.

Elephants

Figure 2

We now suppose that we are looking at an elephant, some distance away, partially occluded by an old penny (about an inch in diameter), somehow suspended in front of the elephant but a good deal nearer to our face, to our eyes, than the elephant. The small penny occludes a good part of the large elephant. So what does LWS-R make of this? A penny, incidentally, brought to us here by the recently discovered ‘remove background’ tool lurking in the depth’s of Microsoft’s Powerpoint. With the white spike right being the end of the left tusk.

We suppose that the penny, the elephant and the background are on the same visual layer of LWS-R.

We suppose that we are looking straight at the penny and that the central part of the image above can be mapped onto our plane patch of cortex without too much distortion, without the sort of distortion and trickery involved in mapping the whole hemisphere of the visual field onto that plane patch of cortex. Think of all the different projections used in atlases of the world, for which, for example, see reference 2.

Our interest is in the subjective experience of the relative size of the penny and the elephant. Does this experience go beyond experiencing the visual, the pixels as it were? Which might be quantified by the amount of electrical activity generated on our patch by the corresponding layer objects on the visual layer. But is there more? It may well be that most of the time we are not consciously aware of size at all. But sometimes we are aware of size, perhaps because the question of relative size has popped into mind, either by a question from the outside or otherwise. An awareness which is perhaps short-lived, not extending much beyond the short period of time needed to vocalise or sub-vocalise the answer. 

So how does LWS-R express the answer to the question about size when it is asked and answered? We are not so interested in how the brain at large gets to that answer, right or wrong. Or with how the compiler decides whether or not to get the answer ready, in the wings as it were, just in case it is needed in consciousness. Rather, how is this information expressed when it is there?

The penny and what is left of the elephant both occupy roughly the same amount of space on our patch, that is to say the image of the penny on the patch is about the same size as that of what is left of the elephant. We suppose that, in the absence of too much of the distortion noted above, if one could look at the patch with a suitable microscope, one would recognise both penny and elephant; quite possibly a bit distorted, but roughly what you see above. This follows from the hypothesised topical organisation of our patch. Continuity also requires these two images to be adjacent, to abut each other. 

Note that the penny is clearly in front of the elephant, although we have yet to define what has been done to make this so. In LWS-N, the scheme was that the relative position of two objects was marked by the way that their shape nets abutted. Here the scheme might be that order is marked by linkage through column objects and a composite object. But that is another matter: size is the present concern, not position.

So, as far as this visual layer goes, the penny and the elephant are perceived as being of the same size, although one might be louder, as it were, than the other. In any event, we probably know that they are not the same size, know in the sense that we will come up with the right answer if prompted. We probably know that the penny is a lot nearer than the elephant and that live elephants are a lot bigger than pennies. On the hypothesis that we are conscious of these matters, at least some of the time, where does this extra information live?

Figure 3

Remember that we have defined our visual layer objects and their regions. In this case, a penny, a suspending thread, an elephant, the foreground and the background, this last possibly split into three: bush, mountain and sky. These visual layer objects might be thought of as an unlabelled diagram projected into consciousness. The raw pixels have been processed into layer objects and regions, have had some structure superimposed on them, but the labels for those layer objects and regions are somewhere else. We associate to the common practise, snapped above from reference 9, of having quite elaborate descriptions attached to diagrams. Descriptions which are not part of the picture but which are linked to the picture.

The LWS-R proposition is that object relations, in this case telling us that this object is larger than that object, are stored on some other layer, with the arguments of those relations linked to the corresponding visual layer objects by column objects.

Note that in real life, some object relations can be deduced from small movements of the head. When the penny is in front of the elephant small movements of the head will result in the more or less fixed image of the penny moving slightly over the changing image of what is left of the elephant, rather than vice-versa. But this is how we and our brain get the information: the job of LWS-R is to express that information with the rather different tools at its disposal.

Note also that in real life, range information can be deduced from binocular vision. Information that the brain needs to extract if LWS-R is to use it in its monocular world.

Figure 4

For a refresher on the sort of plane geometry suggested above see reference 4. While in thinking about size, the brain might start with solid angles, for which see reference 3. But other things being equal, something with a big solid angle is big: in which connection, we can say that the solid angle subtended at the eyes by the penny is about the same as that subtended by the elephant. However, there is the complication that for a given solid angle, the image on the retina of an object on the periphery is bigger than that of an image at the centre – with the figure above – a vertical section through an eyeball, lens centre right – being suggestive rather than robust. 

In any event, solid angle alone does not tell us everything that we want to know about size, even if the brain is clever enough to work out solid angle from what it has got on the retina. Noting here that while the brain probably can do the necessary sums, it is also true that it prefers not to have to, preferring rather to look straight at things which are important. Think of the pointing of a hunting dog. Think of squaring up to things.

All things considered, it seems unlikely that the subjective knowledge about relative size is stored on our visual layer: rather we have one of the object relations mentioned above, stored on some other layer. Which we illustrate in the figure following.

Figure 5

In this example, adapted from reference 6, LWS-R wants to express the fact that Object A is actually smaller than Object C, despite appearances. Which it does by introducing the link object containing something equivalent to ‘is larger than’. To which link we need to add direction, which we say in this case, by convention should run from object C to object A. perhaps in the form of waves of activation traversing object B in that direction.

Note that layer A is different from layer B and layer C is different from layer B, but layer A may well be the same as layer C.

So while at reference 6, we were concerned that the layer objects introduced to carry the column objects needed to create composite objects might themselves be part of the subjective experience, in this example, we need object B to be part of the subjective experience. 

Speculating, it may be that this object B is some derivative of the motor actions, the many commands to face, throat, mouth and tongue, needed to say ‘is larger than’.

Some theory

First, we define some terms.

Subject. The person having a subjective experience, the person (or possibly the animal) who is conscious. Rather than the ego which we came across in some old papers recently

Experience. Shorthand for the subjective experience, the subjective conscious experience

Data. The totality of material in the frames of LWS-N under consideration

Selection. The material in the frame of LWS-N under consideration which is making a contribution to the subjective experience. Often a proper subset of the data

Thing. Something out in the real world. Often something simple like a rabbit or a saucepan. Sometimes something more complicated or diffuse like a cloud, a small flock of birds feeding on the lawn or a small crowd of people playing football

Object. Whatever it is that the subject is attending to. Most of the time some thing out there in the real world. Or some things in the case that the subject is attending to the relations between them. Sometimes to some thing which has been internalised. Sometimes to something quite abstract

Image. The collection of material in the frame of LWS-N which goes into to the experience of the object. This will usually involve more than one layer and will often include part of the visual layer. Not very well defined at the margins. The image is part of, is a subset of the selection, usually a proper subset.

With our to-do list including mapping these terms onto those used by Langer in her discussion of the logic of signs and symbols in chapter 3 of reference 13. Where, roughly speaking, a sign announces something – in the way that the gong used to announce dinner – and signs being something that many animals can cope with – while a symbol merely evokes something, brings it to mind, rather than announcing its imminent arrival, in the flesh, as it were.

We do not attempt tight definitions but we hope, nevertheless, that these special words will be helpful. Note the use of ‘frames’ in the plural and see Figure 13 in the ‘other matters’ section below for a proposed loosening of the rule that the frame is the indivisible unit of consciousness.

We note also that it is quite hard, although not impossible, to attend to two things which are, visually, a long way apart. Two things which are next to each other, or which at least appear to be next to each other are much easier.

Second, we define a process.

Figure 6

Suppose some thing out in the real world catches our subject’s eye. Becomes the object of the moment, a moment which might translate into one or more frames of consciousness. The figure above suggests various stages that the subject might go through with this object: stages which are optional, which overlap, which are of varying duration and which might or might not be conscious, might or might not be part of the experience. But possibly a major part, at least for a while. 

The is the reflex option is there for emergencies and is usually taken when the object is damaging or threatening. Clear and present danger to use a bit of jargon from the US. The fastest possible action is needed, even at risk of making a mistake. Some people can be trained to block this option, at least a lot of the time: reflexes might be fast but they are too often wrong.

The next option is identification. A preliminary identification of the object, speed still being important here, in case of danger. Nevertheless, a preliminary identification which the subject likes to verify, it being all too easy, for one reason or another, to make mistakes. So having decided, for example, that the object is an elephant on the basis of its large ears, for verification the subject looks around for some tusks and the distinctive tail. So having heard the dinner gong, or what sounded like the dinner gong, one might look at one’s watch to make sure that it was about the right time for dinner.

Then the subject might just attend to the object and not be consciously thinking about anything. There may be nothing in the experience apart from that of attending. Alternatively, the subject may be aware, for example, that verification is going on, but not be aware of the details of that verification. Perhaps until something is uncovered which falsifies the identification. Then that something will become part of the experience.

Exploration is what happens when the subject has decided what the object is. Is reasonably sure that the object is an elephant and is giving it the once over to see how it compares to elephants in general. Is this one an outlier or is it a pretty regular elephant? Does it have any unusual features? Is it a threat or an opportunity?

The subject may decide to do something in response to or about the elephant. Perhaps move towards it to take a better look. Alternatively, the subject may decide that the elephant is of no particular interest and attention moves on. A decision which might well be taken without the elephant ever having made it to consciousness.

With our to-do list including mapping these stages onto the takes and frames of LWS-R. For which again, see Figure 13 below.

Other devices

Making use of binocular vision to see around small object near the eyes

Figure 7

The figure above – a view from above of the two eyes of a human looking at a boat – suggests that one or other eye can see more or less all of an object apparently occluded by another, much nearer object.

So to this extent the brain can chose what to project into consciousness. Between them, the eyes are seeing both the occluding object and the occluded object. Or in the figure above, most of it.

Making use of brain power to blot out objects

Figure 8

The thought here is that the brain decides to delete the bird at some point in the vision processing pathway, perhaps thinking it is noise rather than signal, just patching over the hole with something plausible. In this sky, patched with a neighbouring bit of sky in Powerpoint. No doubt someone with Photoshop could do a better job, a proper invisible mend.

Note, incidentally, how the various extra layers of image processing have changed the appearance of the clouds bottom left. Another reminder that images are not the same as facts on the ground. Indeed, that there are no visual facts on the ground: colour is an artefact of the brain, or in this case, the combination of camera and computer.

Alternatively, the brain seems to be quite good at papering overing things it has not got around to noticing. Perhaps the image which reaches consciousness is the result of sampling, and sometimes the sampling is not very representative and features get missed. So one can be looking at a clear blue sky and all of a sudden a bird or an aeroplane pops into focus, a bird or an aeroplane which was there, unseen, all along.

We associate to the management training film clips in which people taking their basketball seriously fail to notice the gorilla wandering about the pitch.

Making use of brain power to vary the sizes of elements of images

Here the brain varies the subjective size of foreground objects according to their importance. A continuous deformation of the original image, a deformation which preserves sight lines but which does change the relative sizes of things. Deformations which are beyond our modest skills with Powerpoint. 

Attention

Rather different, we might be attending to the penny rather than the elephant, or vice-versa. 

This information could be coded on the visual layer by means of the amplitude of the signals, of the travelling waves involved, perhaps by saying that what we mean by attention is the sum of amplitude over the area of the layer object or region in question. And it may well be that most of the time, one particular layer object or region is getting most of the attention, with the rest getting little if any. 

One might also argue that attention is something going on elsewhere in the brain and that this sum of amplitude, a product of the compiler, is a symptom of that attention, rather than actually amounting to attention in itself.

What does the subject bring to the party?

By which we mean, what sort of value does the subject, the person having the subjective experience, add to the inbound visual information from what we are calling the object? In which connection we note the common saying about works of art, certainly old masters and the plays of Shakespeare, that the more one puts in, the more one gets out. The naive consumer does not get to consume very much at all.

Part of this is the subject’s knowledge of this objects and objects like it. But another part is the state of mind of the subject. Another is the relationship between the subject and the object. 

Then we might divide the problem in time: where are we in the sense of the stages of Figure 6 above. Are we at assessment or action?

So suppose we present Figure 2 above to the subject – as an image, not for real – and ask him about the relative sizes of the foreground object, unnamed, and the elephant? What does the subject need to know in order to decide what the object was and how big it was. 

First, he needs to know enough about coins to work out that this is one. Second, he needs to know that coins are generally quite small. Third, he needs to work out that a large cardboard cut-out of a coin is unlikely. Although this would get more likely if there were any signs of filming or staging equipment or personnel in the background.

Suppose we present a picture of a bird to the subject and give him a multiple choice box to check? If the choice was coot or cuckoo, this might not be too difficult, but what about whinchat or stonechat? It seems likely that a subject who was an expert on birds would see more, would have a richer experience, than a subject who was not. The expert would know to look for the tricky little patch of red underneath the tail. Or whatever.

But if the subject was from E. F. Benson’s Mapp & Lucia and had recently participated in a disastrous full-dress tableau of ‘Rule Britannia’, the subject might have a very strong emotional response to the image on the coin, to the exclusion of the elephant and of any sober consideration of matters of size.

There will be interaction between perception and the percept. What we see is strongly influenced by what we think it is and we humans have the ability to project all sorts of things onto all sorts of other things, particularly things like clouds and the more or less random dots of paint on the floors of trains on the London undergound. It which latter case, it is very easy, for example, to make two round dots of the same colour, reasonably close together, into the gazing eyes of an animal or person. A very important matter in the jungle, both for us and for many of our vertebrate relatives, where every bush might be hiding a predator and their eyes might be all that is visible.

There will be interaction between the state of the subject, the subject’s emotions, desires and intentions, and the perception. Some people, for example, are all too apt to see what they want to see. While other people are all to apt to see what they don’t want to see. A tweeter might oscillate rather violently between being sure he is seeing a whinchat (let us say, very rare) and being sure that he is seeing a stonechat (let us say, very common). 

Then what the subject sees will be influenced by the relation between the subject and object.

If the subject is on a bicycle and the object is a car moving in his direction, the subject ought to be interested in the likely trajectory of the car, relative to himself. A prediction which will be largely based on current speed and direction. He may see little else while he is making this prediction and deciding what, if anything, he needs to do. With the ingredients of this prediction being mostly unconscious, as is the business of predicting itself.

Suppose that the subject is the computer in charge of my car and the object is another car. As with the cyclist, all the computer needs to know is speed, direction and changes in same. Maybe something about the size and shape of the car. In the knowledge that it is getting and can process updates very fast, a lot faster than a human. All of which it can do on the basis of a quite impoverished visual experience. It doesn’t need to know that the car has the very latest paint job.

Suppose that the subject is a hunter and object is a deer, presently stationary, but not quite near enough. That the hunter has already decided that this deer is a worthwhile target. In this case the subject is mainly interested in getting closer to the deer without disturbing it. His thoughts will be on the direction of the wind and on the crackling – or not, preferably not – of the litter of twigs and leaves underfoot.

Suppose that the subject is an animal lover and the object is a deer. An animal lover who is interested in the detail of the appearance of the deer and in what it is doing. Perhaps the texture of its fur and the pretty pattern of its spots. Matters of only peripheral interest to the hunter.

Figure 9

Lastly, suppose that the subject is a farmer and the object is a hay barn, something like that above left, a reproduction of the woodcut at reference 7. The farmer will know all about the construction of barns and all about hay. He will be interested in all the details. Perhaps the condition of the poles holding up the roofs. Is the hay adequately ventilated? Whereas a holiday maker who knew nothing of barns or hay might be taking a more aesthetic interest. Was the ambience properly countrified and rustic? What about all the litter? Were there any visitor facilities in the vicinity? Whereas a baby might see nothing much at all. Just a few vague shapes which did not mean a great deal. But perhaps something to be explored, to be poked or picked up.

The subjective experience of these three subjects is not going to be the same, even if we try to confine ourselves to visual aspects of things. Mainly because they each contribute something different – or in the case of the baby, nothing much at all – to the party.

Note that the baby might be quicker on the uptake about spotting something with eyes, this being more basic to its maintenance and survival. This bit boots up quite early in the process of growing up.

Other matters

Flatness

At reference 6 we suggested that our patch of cortex ought to be more or less flat. In which one might include being more or less spherical, or at least hemispherical. With mapping the visual and audio world onto a hemisphere being a more straightforward business than mapping them onto the plane.

But we do not include, for example, the tight folds of the cerebellum, rather tighter than those of the cerebrum. The point being that on such a tightly folded surface the fields generated by our travelling waves on the various part of our patch would destructively interfere each other.

Triangulation

Figure 10

Figure 11

We have had some reports about the retinal image breaking down a bit, judged retinal as the damage moves around with the eyes. No information about whether it was one or both eyes. With strips of brightly coloured triangles appearing around the centre of the field of vision. Usually wearing off after a short while, minutes rather than hours. We have attempted to suggest something of the sort in the first of the two figures above, although we have quite failed to capture the brightness, whiteness and intensity of the triangles described.

Evidence that triangulation is used, in the way of computer vision programs, somewhere along the human visual pathway? The sort of thing we mean being suggested by the second of the two figures above.

Diagrams

Figure 12

These two sketches being taken from page 69 of Langer’s book at reference 13. Being a neat illustration of how two very simple diagrams of animals can be nearly identical while successfully symbolising two quite different animals, different enough that we are unlikely to confuse them in real life. A trick accomplished by the rabbit having long ears and short tail and the cat having short ears and long tail. Two traits, two properties which are enough to both identify and differentiate these two animals. Noting in passing that diagrams of this sort might have difficulty distinguishing rabbits and hares. 

Quite young children will correctly identify these sketches.

The shape nets of LWS-N would have followed the line drawings directly, usually breaking down the interior into parts, while the drawings implicit in the regional representation of animals in LWS-R would amount to something very similar: perhaps a region for ear, a region for tail and a region for the rest of the body. Perhaps a column link to something saying cat, rabbit or whatever. 

Frames

Figure 13

 At reference 1, the scheme was the consciousness was delivered by the frame, with the understanding that a frame might allow predicted change, say a car moving across the foreground in a predictable way.

We now propose loosening this up a little, incidentally giving the take a clearer role between the scene and the frame. Scene is like a scene in the theatre; it involves a change of scene. Typically a change of places and of persons. Most objects are created for the duration of a scene, although we do allow some object creation at the other levels. A take is a segment of a scene for which the layers are generally fixed, although we do allow some layer creation at frame level. A frame is what is delivered by the compiler, usually making use of a lot of the material which went into the one before. It is only the period of consciousness, at the top of the heap, which really starts from scratch.

And while a frame is often only of the order of a second in duration, it may be a lot longer. Perhaps if attention has really been caught by something which is not itself changing much in time. Perhaps trained meditators work at achieving long frames.

Conclusions

A bit of a medley. But hopefully it has served to flesh out some of the visual byways of LWS-R. 

One outcome is the suggestion that information about the relative position of objects in the visual field, on the stage as it were, be held off-layer in composite objects. Rather than using fiddly properties of the waves which define regions, fiddly properties which would be analogous to those of shape nets developed for LWS-N and of arrays developed for LWS-W. For which last see, for example, Figure 7 of reference 1.

We have also suggested that much of the information which might be supporting the visual layer does not make it to consciousness most of the time. Most of the time we are not conscious of the visually interesting flower being that of a dracaena trifasciata – for which follow the pointer at reference 12.

All this supporting information is becoming transient supporting information. Available to and sometimes in consciousness, but mostly not. And in any event, on supporting layers, rather than included on the visual layer in some more or less tricky way.

References

Reference 1: http://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/an-updated-introduction-to-lws-r.html.

Reference 2: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Map_projection

Reference 3: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Solid_angle

Reference 4: School Geometry: Matriculation Edition – Workman & Cracknell – 1923. For revision on school geometry. A throwback to our own school days.

Reference 5: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fovea_centralis

Reference 6: http://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/column-objects.html

Figure 14

Reference 7: Haybarns at Eemdijk - George Mackley – 1962. Eemdijk being a small place on the River Eem, a little to the east of Amsterdam. According to gmaps, more or less brand new, but maybe the woodcut was derived, worked up, in part at least, from the barn in the middle of the snap above. But be warned! Sixty years ago is a long time and who knows what was to be found, what happened at Eemdijk during the second world war.

Reference 8: Consciousness Without Content: A Look at Evidence and Prospects - Narayanan Srinivasan – 2020.

Reference 9: Architecture and development of olivocerebellar circuit topography - Stacey L. Reeber, Joshua J. White, Nicholas A. George-Jones and Roy V. Sillitoe – 2013.

Reference 10: The Mind Is Flat: The Illusion of Mental Depth and the Improvised Mind - Nick Chater – 2018.

Reference 11: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ubNF9QNEQLA.

Reference 12: https://psmv4.blogspot.com/2020/09/more-flower.html

Reference 13: Philosophy in a new key: A Study in the Symbolism of Reason, Rite, and Art - Langer, S. K. – 1942.

Reference 14: https://psmv3.blogspot.com/2017/01/progress-report-on-descriptive.html

Reference 15: https://www.nationalgalleries.org/. With thanks for Figure 9. Rather better than we could manage with our telephones.

Group search key: sre.